CE  Vol.5 No.10 , June 2014
Attending to Feeling: It May Matter More than You Think
Author(s) Carol R. Aldous*
ABSTRACT
Recent advances in cognitive neuroscience reveal that reasoning and decision-making are comprised of both cognitive (thinking) and non-cognitive (feeling, intuiting) components. Such information is shedding new light on what it means for learners both to think well and to solve problems creatively. Former wisdom held that feeling and emotion interfered with one’s capacity to think: what was required for meaningful learning was for emotion to be set aside in order for novel problems to be solved. However, what was not espoused and what is now becoming increasingly evident are that in the absence of feeling, novel problems are unlikely to be solved at all. This paper documents the findings of a large scale study of middle school students (n = 405) in which both cognitive and non-cognitive components of thinking were measured and their relationships to successful novel problem-solving assessed. The cognitive and non-cognitive measures involved five aspects: a strategic, a systematic, a spatial-verbal, a free-flowing and a feeling approach to reasoning. Contrary to the traditional viewpoint, those learners who employed a feeling approach to reasoning were more likely to solve a novel problem than those who did not. This holds significant import not only for the processes of learning, teaching and successful problem-solving, but also for the practice and process of guidance counseling.



Cite this paper
Aldous, C. (2014) Attending to Feeling: It May Matter More than You Think. Creative Education, 5, 780-796. doi: 10.4236/ce.2014.510091.
References
[1]   Aldous, C. R. (2001). Measuring Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Systems of Reasoning: Some Preliminary Findings. International Education Journal, 2, 1-18.

[2]   Aldous, C. R. (2006). Creativity in Problem Solving: Uncovering Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Systems of Reasoning in the Solving of Novel Mathematics Problems. PhD Thesis Adelaide: The Flinders University of South Australia.

[3]   Aldous, C. R. (2009). The Genesis of New Ideas: Models, Feeling and Solutions. In B. Matthews, & T. Gibbons (Eds.), The Process of Research in Education (pp. 338-370). Adelaide: Shannon Research Press.

[4]   Aldous, C. R. (2012). Creativity, Problem Solving and Feeling. In N. M. Seel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning (Part 3, pp. 843-846). New York, NY: Springer Science.

[5]   Aldous, C. R. (2013). Creativity and Emotions. In E. G. Carayannis (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Creativity, Invention, Innovation and Entrepreneurship (pp. 377-383). New York, NY: Springer Science. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-3858-8_354

[6]   Amabile, T. M. (1983). The Social Psychology of Creativity. New York: Springer-Verleg.

[7]   Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in Context: Update to the Social Psychology of Creativity. Boulder, CO: Westview.

[8]   Anderson, J. R. (1995). Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications (4th ed.). New York: W. H. Freeman.

[9]   Australian Mathematics Trust (AMT) (2001). Mathematics Challenge for Young Australians: Teacher’s Reference Book for Primary, Junior & Intermediate Stages. An Activity of the Australian Mathematics Olympiad Committee: A Subcommittee of the Australian Mathematics Trust in Association with the Australian Academy of Science and the University of Canberra. Canberra: AMT Publishing.

[10]   Bechara, A., & Damasio, H. (1997). Deciding Advantageously before Knowing the Advantageous Strategy. Science, 275, 1293-1295.

[11]   Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). The Iowa Gambling Task and the Somatic Marker Hypothesis: Some Questions and Answers. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 159-162.

[12]   Biasutti, M. (2011). Flow and Optimal Experience. In M. A. Runco, & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Creativity (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 522-528). London: Academic Press.

[13]   Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and Communication. New York: Permagon.

[14]   Carroll, J. B. (1963). A Model of School Learning. Teachers College Record, 64, 723-733.

[15]   Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511571312

[16]   Cauffman, E., Shulman, E. P., Steinberg, L., Claus, C., Banich, M. T., Graham, S., & Woolard, J. (2010). Age Differences in Affective Decision Making as Indexed by Performance on the Iowa Gambling Task. Developmental Psychology, 46, 193- 207.

[17]   Cohen, J. (1992). A Power Primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159.

[18]   Collins, M. A., & Amabile, T. M. (1999). Motivation and Creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of Creativity (pp. 297-312). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

[19]   Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1991). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York: Harper Perennial.

[20]   Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Emotion Reason and the Human Brain. London: Papermac.

[21]   Damasio, A. R. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens. London: William Heinemann.

[22]   Dansky, J. L. (1999). Play. In M. A. Runco, & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Creativity (Vol. 2, pp. 393-408). London: Academic Press.

[23]   Das, J. P. (2003). A Look at Intelligence as Cognitive Neuropsychological Processes: Is Luria still Relevant? Japanese Journal of Special Education, 40, 631-647.

[24]   Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2001). Self-Determination Theory: An Approach to Human Motivation and Personality: Intrinsic Motivation Inventory(IMI). Rochester: Rochester University. http://www.psych.rochester.edu/SDT/measures/intrins.html

[25]   Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., & Tsivkin, S. (1999). Sources of Mathematical Thinking: Behavioral and Brain-Imaging Evidence. Science, 284, 970-974.

[26]   Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the Cognitive and the Psychodynamic Unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709-724. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.49.8.709

[27]   Epstein, S. (1998). Constructive Thinking the Key to Emotional Intelligence. Westport, CT: Praeger.

[28]   Eubanks, D. L., Murphy, S. T., & Mumford, M. D. (2010). Intuition as an Influence on Creative Problem Solving: The Effects of Intuition, Positive Affect and Training. Creativity Research Journal, 22, 170-184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2010.481513

[29]   Evans, M., Hastings, N., & Peacock, B. (2000). Statistical Distributions (3rd ed.). New York: Wiley.

[30]   Falk, F. R., & Miller, N. B. (1992). A Primer for Soft Modeling. Arkon, OH: The University of Arkon Press.

[31]   Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative Cognition: Theory, Research and Application. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

[32]   French, J. W., Ekstrom, R. B., & Price, L. A. (1963). Manual for Kit of Reference Tests for Cognitive Factors. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.

[33]   Ghiselin, B. (1963). The Creative Process and Its Relation to the Identification of Creative Talent. In C. W. Taylor, & F. Barron (Eds.), Scientific Creativity: Its Recognition and Development (pp. 355-364). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

[34]   Haenlein, M., & Kaplan, A. M. (2004). A Beginner’s Guide to Partial Least Squares Analysis. Understanding Statistics, 3, 283-297.

[35]   Immordino-Yang, M. H., & Faeth, M. (2010). The Role of Emotion and Skilled Intuition in Learning. In D. A. Sousa (Ed.), Mind Brain and Education (pp. 69-84). Bloomington, IN: Solution Tree Press.

[36]   Jeffries, D. A. (2011). Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Processes: Students’ Perceptions about and Employment of Approaches to Novel Mathematics Problem Solving. Honors, Thesis, Adelaide: The Flinders University of South Australia.

[37]   Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking Fast and Slow. Maryborough: Allen Lane.

[38]   Katz, A. N., & Hussey, K. A. (2011). Psycholinguistics. In M. A. Runco, & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Creativity (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 271-278). London: Academic Press.

[39]   Lietz, P. (1995). Changes in Reading Comprehension across Cultures and over Time. PhD. Thesis, Adelaide: The Flinders University of South Australia.

[40]   Lubart, T. (2001). Models of the Creative Process: Past Present and Future. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 295-308. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15326934CRJ1334_07

[41]   Mooney, R. L. (1963). A Conceptual Model for Integrating Four Approaches to the Identification of Creative Talent. In C. W. Taylor, & F. Barron (Eds.), Scientific Creativity: Its Recognition and Development (pp. 331-340). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

[42]   Noonan, R. D., & Wold, H. (1988). Partial Least Squares Path Analysis. In J. P. Keeves (Ed.), Educational Research Methodology and Measurement (pp. 710-716). Oxford: Pergamon Press.

[43]   Poincare, H. (1924). The Foundations of Science (G. B. Halstead, Trans.). Science Press.

[44]   Richardson, J. T. E. (1999). Imagery. Hove: Psychology Press.

[45]   Rigdon, E. E. (2012). Rethinking Partial Least Squares Path Modeling: In Praise of Simple Methods. Long Range Planning, 45, 341-358. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lrp.2012.09.010

[46]   Ringle, C. M., Sarstedt, M., Schlittgen, R., & Taylor, C. R. (2013). PLS Path Modelling and Evolutionary Segmentation. Journal of Business Research, 66, 1318-1324.

[47]   Russ, S. W., & Christian, K. M. (2011). Play. In M. R. Runco, & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Creativity (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 238-243). London: Academic Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-375038-9.00176-X

[48]   Sellin, N. (1990). PLSPath Version 3.01 Program Manual. Unpublished Manuscript, Hamburg.

[49]   Sellin, N., & Keeves, J. P. (1997). Path Analysis with Latent Variables. In J. P. Keeves (Ed.), Educational Research, Methodology, and Measurement: An International Handbook (pp. 633-640). Oxford: Pergamon Press.

[50]   Shaw, M. P. (1989). The Eureka Process: A Structure for the Creative Experience in Science and Engineering. Creativity Research Journal, 2, 286-298. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400418909534325

[51]   Sloman, S. A. (1996). The Empirical Case for Two Systems of Reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3-22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.3

[52]   Stone, M. (1974). Cross-Validatory Choice and Assessment of Statistical Predictions. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B, 36, 111-147.

[53]   Vogt, W. P. (1999). Dictionary of Statistics and Methodology: A Non-Technical Guide for the Social Sciences (2nd ed.). London: Sage Publications.

[54]   Wallas, G. (1926). The Art of Thought. London: Jonathan Cape Ltd.

[55]   Ward, T. H. (2011). Problem Solving. In M. R. Runco, & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Creativity (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 254-260). London: Academic Press.

[56]   Werner, N. S., Duschek, S., & Schandry, R. (2009). Relationships between Affective States and Decision-Making. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 74, 259-265.

 
 
Top