[1] Albaret, J. M., & Thon, B. (1998). Differential Effects of Task Complexity on Contextual Interference in a Drawing Task. Acta Psychologica, 100, 9-24.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(98)00022-5
[2] Barreiros, J., Figueiredo, T., & Godinho, M. (2007). The Contextual Interference Effect in Applied Settings. European Physical Education Review, 13, 195-208.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1356336X07076876
[3] Bastos, F. H., Marinovic, W., De Rugy, A., & Tani, G. (2013). Prior Knowledge of Final Testing Improves Sensorimotor Learning through Self-Scheduled Practice. Human Movement Science, 32, 192-202.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2012.11.008
[4] Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57, 289-300.
[5] Boutin, A., & Blandin, Y. (2010). Cognitive Underpinnings of Contextual Interference during Motor Learning. Acta Psychologica, 135, 233-239.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.004
[6] Brashers-Krug, T., Shadmehr, R., & Bizzi, E. (1996). Consolidation in Human Motor Memory. Nature, 382, 252-255.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/382252a0
[7] Corrêa, U. C., & Tani, G. (2005). Practice Structure and Adaptive Process in Motor Learning: For a New Approach of Practice. In Motor Behavior: Learning and Development (pp. 141-161). Rio de Janeiro: Guanabara Koogan.
[8] Guadagnoli, M. A., & Lee, T. D. (2004). Challenge Point: A Framework for Conceptualizing the Effects of Various Practice Conditions in Motor Learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 212-224.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/JMBR.36.2.212-224
[9] Kantak, S. S., Sullivan, K. J., Fisher, B. E., Knowlton, B. J., & Winstein, C. J. (2010). Neural Substrates of Motor Memory Consolidation Depend on Practice Structure. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 923-925.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2596
[10] Lee, T. D., & Magill, R. A. (1983). The Locus of Contextual Interference in Motor-Skill Acquisition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 730-746.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.9.4.730
[11] Lee, T. D., & Magill, R. A. (1985). Can Forgetting Facilitate Skill Acquisition? In R. B. W. and I. M. F. David Goodman (Org.), Advances in Psychology (Vol. 27, pp. 3-22). North-Holland.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62528-5
[12] Lee, T. D., Swinnen, S. P., & Serrien, D. J. (1994). Cognitive Effort and Motor Learning. Quest, 46, 328-344.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00336297.1994.10484130
[13] Lee, T. D., & Weeks, D. J. (1987). The Beneficial Influence of Forgetting on Shorterm Retention of Movement Information. Human Movement Science, 6, 233-245.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(87)90014-5
[14] Magill, R. A., & Hall, K. G. (1990). A Review of the Contextual Interference Effect in Motor Skill Acquisition. Human Movement Science, 9, 241-289.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(90)90005-X
[15] Massigli, M., Nunes, M. E. de S., Freudenheim, A. M., & Corrêa, U. C. (2011). Practice Structure and Ecological Validity on the Adaptive Process of Motor Learning. Revista Brasileira de Educacao Física e Esporte, 25, 39-48.
[16] Moreno, F. J., Avila, F., Damas, J., García, J. A., Luis, V., Reina, R., & Ruíz, A. (2003). Contextual Interference in Learning Precision Skills. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 97, 121-128.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.2003.97.1.121
[17] Naimo, M. A., Zourdos, M. C., Wilson, J. M., Kim, J.-S., Ward, E. G., Eccles, D. W., & Panton, L. B. (2013). Contextual Interference Effects on the Acquisition of Skill and Strength of the Bench Press. Human Movement Science, 32, 472-484.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2013.02.002
[18] Ollis, S., Button, C., & Fairweather, M. (2005). The Influence of Professional Expertise and Task Complexity upon the Potency of the Contextual Interference Effect. Acta Psychologica, 118, 229-244.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2004.08.003
[19] Porter, J. M., & Magill, R. A. (2010). Systematically Increasing Contextual Interference Is Beneficial for Learning Sport Skills. Journal of Sports Sciences, 28, 1277-1285.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2010.502946
[20] R Core Team (2013). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (Version 3.0.2). Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
[21] Shea, C. H., Lai, Q., Black, C., & Park, J.-H. (2000). Spacing Practice Sessions across Days Benefits the Learning of Motor Skills. Human Movement Science, 19, 737-760. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0167-9457(00)00021-X
[22] Shea, J. B., & Morgan, R. L. (1979). Contextual Interference Effects on the Acquisition, Retention, and Transfer of a Motor Skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 179-187.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.5.2.179
[23] Shea, J. B., & Wright, D. L. (1991). When Forgetting Benefits Motor Retention. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 62, 293-301.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02701367.1991.10608726
[24] Shea, J. B., & Zimny, S. T. (1983). Context Effects in Memory and Learning Movement Information. In R. A. Magill (Org.), Advances in Psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 345-366). North-Holland.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61998-6
[25] Silva, J. A. de O., Araujo, U. O., Tani, G., & Corrêa, U. C. (2009). Practice Schedule and Task Complexity on the Adaptive Process of Motor Learning. Revista da Educacao Física/UEM, 20, 313-323.
http://dx.doi.org/10.4025/reveducfis.v20i3.6881
[26] Souza, T. O., Santos, J. J., Torriani-Pasin, C., & Corrêa, U. C. (2013). O efeito da interferência contextual na aquisicao de habilidades motoras e as dificuldades funcional e nominal da tarefa. Motricidade, 9, 57-69.
http://dx.doi.org/10.6063/motricidade.9(3).763
[27] Stickgold, R. (2013). Parsing the Role of Sleep in Memory Processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 847-853.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.04.002
[28] Travlos, A. K. (2010). Specificity and Variability of Practice, and Contextual Interference in Acquisition and Transfer of an Underhand Volleyball Serve. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 110, 298-312.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.110.1.298-312
[29] Wright, D. L. (1991). The Role of Intertask and Intratask Processing in Acquisition and Retention of Motor Skills. Journal of Motor Behavior, 23, 139-145.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00222895.1991.9942031
[30] Wright, D. L., Li, Y., & Whitacre, C. (1992). The Contribution of Elaborative Processing to the Contextual Interference Effect. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 63, 30-37.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02701367.1992.10607554
[31] Wrisberg, C. A., & Liu, Z. (1991). The Effect of Contextual Variety on the Practice, Retention, and Transfer of an Applied Motor Skill. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 62, 406-412.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02701367.1991.10607541
[32] Wu, W. F. W., Young, D. E., Schandler, S. L., Meir, G., Judy, R. L. M., Perez, J., & Cohen, M. J. (2011). Contextual Interference and Augmented Feedback: Is There an Additive Effect for Motor Learning? Human Movement Science, 30, 10921101.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2011.02.004
[33] Yanci, J., Reina, R., Los Arcos, A., & Camara, J. (2013). Effects of Different Contextual Interference Training Programs on Straight Sprinting and Agility Performance of Primary School Students. Journal of Sports Science & Medicine, 12, 601607.