[1] van Schaik, C.P., Daener, R.O. and Merrill, M.Y. (1999) The Conditions for Tool Use in Primates: Implications for the Evolution of Material Culture. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 719-741.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1999.0304
[2] Bird, C.D. and Emery, N.J. (2009) Insightful Problem Solving and Creative Tool Modification by Captive Nontool-Using Rooks. PNAS, 106, 10370-10375.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0901008106
[3] Weir, A.A.S., Chappel, J. and Kacelnik, A. (2002) Shaping of Hooks in New Caledonian Crow. Science, 297, 981.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1073433
[4] Kacelnik, A. (2009) Tools for Thought or Thoughts for Tools? PNAS, 106, 10071-10072.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0904735106
[5] Jones, T.B. and Kamil, A.C. (1973) Tool-Making and Tool-Using in the Northern Blue Jay. Science, 180, 1076-1078.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.180.4090.1076
[6] Lind, J., Ghirlanda, S. and Enquist, M. (2009) Insight Learning or Shaping? PNAS, 106, E76.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0906120106
[7] Huber, L. and Gajdon, G.K. (2006) Technical Intelligence in Animals: The Kea Model. Animal Cognition, 9, 295-305.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10071-006-0033-8
[8] Kenward, B., Schlögl, C., Rutz, C., Weir, A.A.S., Bugnyar, T. and Kacelnik, A. (2011) On the Evolutionary and Ontogenetic Origins of Tool-Oriented Behaviour in New Caledonian Crows (Corvus moneduloides). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 102, 870-877.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8312.2011.01613.x
[9] Auersperg, A.M.I., von Bayern, A.M.F., Gajdon, G.K., Huber, L. and Kacelnik, A. (2011) Flexibility in Problem Solving and Tool Use of Kea and New Caledonian Crows in a Multi Access Box Paradigm. PLos ONE, 6, e20231.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020231
[10] Auersperg, A.M.I., Szabo, B., von Bayern, A.M.P. and Kacelnik, A. (2012) Spontaneous Innovation in Tool Manufacture and Use in a Goffin’s Cockatoo. Current Biology, 22, R903-R904.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.002
[11] Benson-Amram, S. and Holekamp, K.E. (2012) Innovative Problem Solving by Wild Spotted Hyenas. Proceedings of the Royal Society, London B, 279, 4087-4095.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.1450
[12] Piaget, J. and Inhelder, B. (1969) The Psychology of the Child. Basic Books, New York.
[13] Torigoe, T. (1985) Comparison of Object Manipulation among 74 Species of Non-Human Primates. Primates, 26, 182-194.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02382017
[14] Visalberghi, E. and Fragaszy, D.M. (2006) What Is Challenging about Tool Use? The Capuchin’s Perspective. In: Wasserman, E.A. and Zentall, T.R., Eds., Comparative Cognition. Experimental Explorations of Animal Intelligence, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 529-552.
[15] Lockman, J.J. (2000) A Perception-Action Perspective on Tool Use Evelopment. Child Development, 71, 137-144.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00127
[16] Pepperberg, I.M. and Shive, H.R. (2001) Simultanous Development of Vocal and Physical Object Combinations in a Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus): Bottles Caps, Lids and Labels. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115, 376-384.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.115.4.376
[17] McCowan, B., Marino, L., Vance, E., Walke, L. and Reiss, D. (2000) Bubble Ring Play of Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): Implications for Cognition. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114, 98-106.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.114.1.98
[18] Epstein, R., Kirshnit, C.E., Lanza, R.P. and Rubin, L.C. (1984) “Insight” in the Pigeon: Antecedents and Determinants of an Inteligent Performance. Nature, 308, 61-62.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/308061a0
[19] Emery, N.J. and Clayton, N.S. (2004) The Mentality of Crows: Convergent Evolution of Intelligence in Corvids and Apes. Science, 306, 1903-1907.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1098410
[20] Veit, L. and Nieder, A. (2013) Abstract Rule Neurons in the Endbrain Support Intelligent Behaviour in Corvid Songbirds. Nature Comunications, 4, Article ID: 2878.