WJNS  Vol.4 No.1 , February 2014
Dissociation of the N400 component between linguistic and non-linguistic processing: A source analysis study
ABSTRACT


The N400 component is commonly associated with the detection of linguistic incongruity. A few studies have shown that the N400 can also be elicited by non-linguistic stimuli. Different spatiotemporal patterns were observed between the typical Linguistic N400 and the Non-linguistic N400, suggesting distinct brain generators. The aim of this study was to investigate the presence of an N400 in response to linguistic and non-linguistic stimuli, and to specify anatomical sources of both N400s using a novel analysis method: the Bayesian Model Averaging (BMA) distributed source model. Picture-word and environmental soundpicture associations, either congruent or incongruent, were presented to ten young healthy adults while highdensity ERP recordings were made. Standard electrophysiological analyses confirmed that the N400 was not specific to linguistic incongruity but was also elicited by environmental sound-picture incongruities. Different topographic distributions were obtained for the Linguistic N400 and Non-linguistic N400. BMA analysis showed that the Linguistic N400 generators were mostly located in the left superior temporal gyrus, whereas the sources of the Non-linguistic N400 were identified mostly in the right middle and superior temporal gyri. Detection of linguistic incongruities recruited cerebral areas commonly associated with language processing, whereas non-linguistic incongruities recruited right cerebral regions usually associated with auditory processing of non-linguistic stimuli. The Linguistic and Non-linguistic N400s appear to be elicited by similar cognitive mechanisms assumed by different cerebral areas depending on the type of material to be processed. The present findings support the existence of parallel pathways for the processing of linguistic and non-linguistic incongruities.



Cite this paper
Gallagher, A. , Béland, R. , Vannasing, P. , Bringas, M. , Sosa, P. , Trujillo-Barreto, N. , Connolly, J. and Lassonde, M. (2014) Dissociation of the N400 component between linguistic and non-linguistic processing: A source analysis study. World Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 25-39. doi: 10.4236/wjns.2014.41004.
References
[1]   Brandeis, D. and Lehmann, D. (1986) Event-related potentials of the brain and cognitive processes: Approaches and applications. Neuropsychologia, 24, 151-168.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(86)90049-7

[2]   Auchterlonie, S., Phillips, N.A., Chertkow, H., 2002. Behavioural and electrical brain measures of semantic priming in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: Implications for access failure versus deterioration hypotheses. Brain and Cognition, 48, 264-267.

[3]   Kutas, M., Federmeier, K.D. (2011) Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621-647.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123

[4]   Phillips, N.A. and Lesperance, D. (2003) Breaking the waves: Age differences in electrical brain activity when reading text with distractors. Psychology and Aging, 18, 126-139. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.18.1.126

[5]   Kutas, M. and Hillyard, S.A. (1980) Event-related brain potentials to semantically inappropriate and surprisingly large words. Biological Psychology, 11, 99-116.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(80)90046-0

[6]   Kutas, M. and Hillyard, S.A. (1980) Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203-205.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(80)90046-0

[7]   Kutas, M. and Hillyard, S.A. (1984) Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 307, 161-163.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/307161a0

[8]   Holcomb, P.J. (1993) Semantic priming and stimulus degradation: Implications for the role of the N400 in language processing. Psychophysiology, 30, 47-61.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03204.x

[9]   Kutas, M. and Hillyard, S.A. (1989) An electrophysiological probe of incidental semantic association. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 38-49.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1989.1.1.38

[10]   Connolly, J.F., Byrne, J.M. and Dyman, C.A. (1995) Assessing adult receptive vocabulary with event-related potentials: An investigation of cross-modal and cross-form priming. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 548-565.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01688639508405145

[11]   Bernadis, P., Salillas, E. and Caramelli, N. (2008) Behavioural and neurophysiological evidence of semantic interaction between iconic gestures and words. Cogn Neuropsychology, 25, 1114-1128.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02643290801921707

[12]   Dunn, L.M. and Dunn, L.M. (1981) Peabody picture vocabulary test-revised. American Guidance Service, Circle Pines.

[13]   Ganis, G., Kutas, M. and Sereno, M.I. (1996) The search for “common sense”: An electrophysiological study of the comprehension of words and pictures in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 89-106.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1996.8.2.89

[14]   Holcomb, P.J. and McPherson, W.B. (1994) Event-related potentials reflect semantic priming in an object decision task. Brain and Cognition, 24, 259-276.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1994.1014

[15]   McPherson, W.B. and Holcomb, P.J. (1999) An electrophysiological investigation of semantic priming with pictures of real objects. Psychophysiology, 36, 53-65
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0048577299971196

[16]   Barrett, S.E. and Rugg, M.D. (1989) Event-related potentials and the semantic matching of faces. Neuropsychologia, 27, 913-922.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(89)90067-5

[17]   Bobes, M.A., Valdes-Sosa, M. and Olivares, E (1994) An ERP study of expectancy violation in face perception. Brain and Cognition, 26, 1-22.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1994.1039

[18]   Jemel, B., et al. (1999) Event-related potentials to structural familiar face incongruity processing. Psychophsiology, 36, 437-452.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1994.1039

[19]   Olivares, E. I., Saavedra, C., Trujillo-Barreto, N. J. and Iglesias, J. (2013) Long-term information and distributed neural activation are relevant for the “internal features advantage” in face processing: Electrophysiological and source reconstruction evidence. Cortex, 49, 2735-2747.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.08.001

[20]   Jost, K., Hennighausen, E. and Rosler, F. (2004) Comparing arithmetic and semantic fact retrieval: Effects of problem size and sentence constraint on event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology, 41, 46-59.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.00119_41_1

[21]   Szucs, D. and Soltész, F. (2010) Event-related brain potentials to violations of arithmetic syntax represented by place value structure. Biological Psychology, 84, 354-367.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.04.002

[22]   Cummings, A., CeponienE, R., Koyama, A., Saygin, A.P., Townsend, J. and Dick, F. (2006) Auditory semantic networks for words and natural sounds. Brain Research, 1115, 92-107.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.07.050

[23]   Cummings, A.,Ceponiene, R., Dick, F., Saygin, A.P. and Townsend, J. (2008) A developmental ERP study of verbal and non-verbal semantic processing. Brain Research, 1208, 137-149.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.02.015

[24]   Fogelson, N., Loukas, C., Brown, J. and Brown, P. (2004) A common N400 EEG component reflecting contextual integration irrespective of symbolic form. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115, 1349-1358.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2004.01.010

[25]   Kirmse, U., Jacobsen, T. and Schroger, E. (2009) Familiarity affects environmental sound processing outside the focus of attention: an event-related potential study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120, 887-896.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2009.02.159

[26]   Lebrun, N., et al. (2001) An ERD mapping study of the neurocognitive processes involved in the perceptual and semantic analysis of environmental sounds and words. Cognitive Brain Research, 11, 235-248.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00078-1

[27]   Orgs, G., Lange, K., Dombrowski, J.H. and Heil, M. (2006) Conceptual priming for environmental sounds and words: an ERP study. Brain and Cognition, 62, 267-272.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2006.05.003

[28]   Orgs, G., Lange, K., Dombrowski, J.H. and Heil, M. (2008) N400-effects to task-irrelevant environmental sounds: Further evidence for obligatory conceptual processing. Neuroscience Letters, 436, 133-137.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.03.005

[29]   Polich, J. (1985) N400s from sentences, semantic categories, number and letter strings. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 361-364.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03330184

[30]   Van Petten, C. and Rheinfelder, H. (1995) Conceptual relationships between spoken words and environmental sounds: Event-related brain potential measures. Neuropsychologia, 33, 485-508.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(94)00133-A

[31]   Grigor, J., Van, S., Toller, Behan, J. and Richardson, A. (1999) The effect of odour priming on long latency visual evoked potentials of matching and mismatching objects. Chemical Senses, 24, 137-144.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/chemse/24.2.137

[32]   Sarfarazi, M., Cave, B., Richardson, A., Behan, J. and Sedgwick, E.M. (1999) Visual event related potentials modulated by contextually relevant and irrelevant olfactory primes. Chemical Senses, 24, 145-154.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/chemse/24.2.145

[33]   Kutas, M. and Hillyard, S.A. (1982) The lateral distribution of event-related potentials during sentence processing. Neuropsychologia, 20, 579-590.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(82)90031-8

[34]   Plante, E., Van Petten, C. and Senkfor, A.J. (2000) Electrophysiological dissociation between verbal and nonverbal semantic processing in learning disabled adults. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1669-1684.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00083-X

[35]   De Munck, J.C., van Dijk, B.W. and Spekeijse, H. (1988) Mathematical dipoles are adequate to describe realistic generators of human brain activity. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 35, 960-966.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/10.8677

[36]   Nunez, P.L. (1981) A study of origins of the time dependencies of scalp EEG: I-theoretical basis. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 28, 271-280.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/TBME.1981.324700

[37]   Scherg, M. and von Cramon, D. (1986) Evoked dipole source potentials of the human auditory cortex. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 65, 344360. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0168-5597(86)90014-6

[38]   Scholz, B. and Schwierz, G. (1994) Probability-based current dipole localization from biomagnetic fields. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 41, 735-742.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/10.310089

[39]   Luck, S.J. (2005) An introduction to the event-related potential technique. MIT Press, Cambridge.

[40]   Helenius, P., Salmelin, R., Service, E. and Connolly, J.F. (1998) Distinct time courses of word and context comprehension in the left temporal cortex. Brain, 121, 11331142. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/121.6.1133

[41]   Helenius, P., Salmelin, R., Service, E. and Connolly, J.F. (1999) Semantic cortical activation in dyslexic readers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 535-550.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892999563599

[42]   Simos, P.G., Basile, L.F.H. and Papanicolaou, A.C. (1997) Source localization of the N400 response in a sentencereading paradigm using evoked magnetic fields and magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Research, 762, 29-39.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00349-1

[43]   Makela, A.M., Makinen, V., Nikkila, M., Ilmoniemi, R.J. and Tiitinen, H. (2001) Magnetoencephalographic (MEG) localization of the auditory N400m: Effects of stimulus duration. Neuroreport, 12, 249-253.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200102120-00014

[44]   D’Arcy, R.C.N., Connolly, J.F., Service, E., Hawco, C.S. and Houlihan, M.E. (2004) Separating phonological and semantic processing in auditory sentence processing: A high-resolution event-related brain potential study. Human Brain Mapping, 22, 40-51.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20008

[45]   D’Arcy, R.C.N., Service, E., Connolly, J.F. and Hawco, C.S. (2005) The influence of increased working memory load on semantic neural systems: A high-resolution eventrelated potential study. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 177-191.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.08.007

[46]   Dien, J., Michelson, C.A. and Franklin, M.S. (2010) Separating the visual sentence N400 effect from the P400 sequential expectancy effect: Cognitive and neuroanatomical implications. Brain Research, 1355, 126-140.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.07.099

[47]   Hamm, J.P., Johnson, B.W. and Kirk, I.J. (2002) Comparison of the N300 and N400 ERPs to picture stimuli in congruent and incongruent contexts. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113, 1339-1350.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00161-X

[48]   Silva-Pereyra, J., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Aubert, E., Bosch, J., Galan, L. and Salazar, A. (2003) N400 during lexical decision tasks: A current source localization study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, 2469-2486.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00248-7

[49]   Bolte, J., Schulz, C. and Dobel, C. (2010) Processing of existing, synonymous, and anomalous German derived adjectives: An MEG study. Neuroscience Letters, 469, 107-111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2009.11.054

[50]   Giglio, A.C., Minati, L. and Boggio, P.S. (2013) Throwing the banana away and keeping the peel: Neuroelectric responses to unexpected but physically feasible action endings. Brain Research, 1532, 56-62.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.08.017

[51]   Haan, H., Streb, J., Bien, S. and Rosler, F. (2000) Individual cortical current density reconstructions of the semantic N400 effect: Using a generalized minimum norm model with different constraints (L1 and L2 norm). Human Brain Mapping, 11, 178-192.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-0193(200011)11:3<178::AID-HBM40>3.0.CO;2-0

[52]   Halgren, E., Dhond, R.P., Christensen, N., Van Petten, C., Marinkovic, K., Lewine, J.D. and Dale, A.M. (2002) N400-like magnetoencephalography response modulated by semantic context, word frequency, and lexical class in sentences. Neuroimage, 17, 1101-1116.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1268

[53]   Khateb, A., Pegna, A.J., Landis, T., Mouthon, M.S. and Annoni, J.M. (2010) On the origin of the N400 effects: An ERP waveform and source localization analysis in three matching tasks. Brain Topography, 23, 311-320.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10548-010-0149-7

[54]   Khateb, A., Pegnaa, A.J., Landisc, T., Micheld, C.M., Brunetd, D., Seghierf, M.L. and Annoni, J.M. (2007) Rhyme processing in the brain: An ERP mapping study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 63, 240-250.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.11.001

[55]   Proverbio, A.M., Crotti, N., Zani, A. and Adomi, R. (2009) The role of left and right hemispheres in the comprehension of idiomatic language: An electrical neuroimaging study. BMC Neuroscience, 10, 116.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-10-116

[56]   Qiu, J., Li, H., Chen, A. and Zhang, Q. (2008) The neural basis of analogical reasoning: An event-realted potential study. Neuropsychologia, 46, 3006-3013.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.06.008

[57]   Ren, G.Q., Liu, Y. and Han, Y.C. (2009) Phonological activation in chinese reading: An event-related potential study using low-resolution electromagnetic tomography. Neuroscience, 164, 1623-1631.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.09.029

[58]   Schulz, E., Maurer, U., van der Mark, S., Bucher, K., Brem, S., Martin, E. and Brandeis D. (2008) Impaired semantic processing during sentence reading in children with dyslexia: Combined fMRI and ERP evidence. Neuroimage, 41, 153-168.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.012

[59]   Travis, K.E., et al. (2013) Independence of early speech processing from word meaning. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 2370-2379. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs228

[60]   Trujillo-Barreto, N.J., Aubert-Vázquez, E. and Valdés-Sosa, P.A. (2004) Bayesian Model Averaging in EEG/MEG imaging. Neuroimage, 21, 1300-1319.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.008

[61]   Kotz, S.A., Opitz, B. and Friederici, A.D. (2007) ERP effects of meaningful and non-meaningful sound processing in anterior temporal patients. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 25, 273-284.

[62]   Thierry, G., Giraud, A.L. and Price, C. (2003) Hemispheric dissociation in access to the human semantic system. Neuron, 38, 499-506.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00199-5

[63]   Schnider, A., Benson, D.F., Alexander, D.N. and SchniderKlaus, A. (1994) Non-verbal environmental sound recognition after unilateral hemispheric stroke. Brain, 17, 281-287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/117.2.281

[64]   Opitz, B., Mecklinger, A. and Friederici, A.D. (2006) Functional asymmetry of human prefrontal cortex: Encoding and retrieval of verbally and nonverbally coded information. Learning Memory, 7, 85-96.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.7.2.85

[65]   Thierry, G. and Price, C. (2006) Dissociating verbal and nonverbal conceptual processing in the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1018-1028.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.6.1018

[66]   Zatorre, R.J., Bouffard, M. and Belin, P. (2004) Sensitivity to auditory object features in human temporal neocortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 3637-3642.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5458-03.2004

[67]   Hoeting, J.A., Madigan, D., Raftery, A.E. and Volinsky, C.T. (1999) Bayesian model averaging: A tutorial. Statistical Science, 14, 382-417.

[68]   MacKay, D.J.C. (1992) Bayesian interpolation. Neural Computation, 4, 415-447.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/neco.1992.4.3.415

[69]   Pascual-Marqui, R.D., Michel, C.M. and Lehmann, D. (1994) Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: A new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 49-65.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-8760(84)90014-X

[70]   Dunn, L.M., Thériault-Whalen, C.M. and Dunn, L.M. (1993) échelle de vocabulaire en images Peabody. PSYCAN, Toronto.

[71]   Alario, F.X. and Ferrand, L. (1999) A set of 400 pictures standardized for French: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, and age of acquisition. Behavior Research Methods, 31, 531-552.

[72]   Bonin, P., Peereman, R., Malardier, N., Méot, A. and Chalard, M. (2003) A new set of 299 pictures standardized for French for name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, visual complexity, image variability and age of acquisition. Behavior Research Methods, 35, 158-167.

[73]   Chalard, M., Bonin, P., Méot, A., Boyer, B. and Fayol, M. (2003) Objective age-of-acquisition (AoA) norms for a set of 230 object names in French: Relationship with psycholinguistic variables, the English data from Morrison et al. (1997), and naming latencies. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 15, 209-245.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09541440244000076

[74]   Ferrand, L., Grainger, J. and New, B. (2002) Normes d’age d’acquisition pour 400 mots monosyllabiques. Notes méthodologiques sur www.lexique.org

[75]   Gratton, G., Coles, M.G., Donchin, E. (1983) A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55, 468-484.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9

[76]   McCarthy, G. and Wood, C.C. (1985) Scalp distribution of event-related potentials: An ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 62, 203-208.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0168-5597(85)90015-2

[77]   Urbach, T.P. and Kutas, M. (2006) Interpreting eventrelated brain potential (ERP) distributions: Implications of baseline potentials and variability with application to amplitude normalization by vector scaling. Biological Psychology, 72, 333-343.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.11.012

[78]   Rush, S. and Driscoll, D.A. (1969) EEG electrode sensitivity-An application of reciprocity. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 16, 15-22.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/TBME.1969.4502598

[79]   Riera, J.J., Fuentes, M.E., Valdes, P.A. and Oharriz, Y. (1997) Theoretical basis of the EEG spline inverse solutions for spherical head model. Biomedical Technology, 42, 219-222.

[80]   Riera, J., Valdes, P., Fuentes, M.E. and Oharriz, Y. (1997) Explicit Backus and Gilbert EEG inverse solution for spherical head model. Biomedical Technology, 42, 216-218.

[81]   Benjamini, Y. and Hochberg, Y. (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: A practica1 and powerful approach to multiple testing. Royal Statistic Society Series B (Methodological), 57, 289-300.

[82]   Besson, M. and Kutas, M. (1993) The many facets of repetition: A cued-recall and event-related potential analysis of repeating words in same versus different sentence contexts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 19, 1115-1133.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.19.5.1115

[83]   Besson, M., Kutas, M. and Van Petten, C. (1992) An event-related potential (ERP) analysis of semantic congruity and repetition effects in sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 132-149.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1992.4.2.132

[84]   Daltrozzo, J. and Schon, D. (2009) Conceptual processing in music as revealed by N400 effects on words and musical targets. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1882-1892. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21113

[85]   Painter, J.G. and Koelsch, S. (2011) Can out-of-context musical sounds convey meaning? An ERP study on the processing of meaning in music. Psychophysiology, 48, 645-655. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01134.x

[86]   Willems, R.M., Ozyurek, A. and Hagoort, P. (2008) Seeing and Hearing Meaning: ERP and fMRI Evidence of Word versus Picture Integration into a Sentence Context. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1235-1249.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20085

[87]   Lau, E.F., Phillips, C. and Poeppel, D. (2008) A cortical network for semantics: (de)constructing the N400. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 9, 920-933.

[88]   Fujii, T., Fukatsu, R., Watabe, S., Ohnuma, A., Teramura, K., Kimura, I., Saso, S. and Kogure, K. (1990) Auditory sound agnosia without aphasia following a right temporal lesion. Cortex, 26, 263-268.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80355-3

[89]   Alkire, M.T., Haier, R.J., Fallon, J.H. and Cahill, L. (1998) Hippocampal, but not amygdala, activity at encoding correlates with long-term, free recall of nonemotional information. PNAS, 95, 14506-14510.

[90]   Young, M.P. and Rugg, M.D. (1992) Word frequency and multiple repetition as determinants of the modulation of event-related potentials in a semantic classification task. Psychophysiology, 29, 664-676.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1992.tb02044.x

[91]   Doyle, M.C., Rugg, M.D. and Wells, T. (1996) A comparison of the electrophysiological effects of formal and repetition priming. Psychophysiology, 33,132-147.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb02117.x

[92]   Aramaki, M., Marie, C., Kronland-Martinet, R., Ystad, S. and Besson, M. (2010) Sound categorization and conceptual; priming for nonlinguistic ans linguistic sounds. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2555-2569.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21398

 
 
Top