References
[1] Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252-1265.
[2] Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 351-355.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x
[3] Blaine, B., & Crocker, J. (1995). Religiousness, race, and psychological well-being: Exploring social psychological mediators. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1031-1041.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/01461672952110004
[4] Cahn, B. R., & Polich, J. (2013). Meditation States and Traits: EEG, ERP, and Neuroimaging Studies. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1, 48-96.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/2326-5523.1.S.48
[5] Chan, D., & Woollacott, M. (2007). Effects of level of meditation experience on attentional focus: Is the efficiency of executive or orienttation networks improved? Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 13, 651-657.
[6] Clark, K. M., Friedman, H. S., & Martin, L. R. (1999). A longitudinal study of religiosity and mortality risk. Journal of Health Psychology, 4, 381-391.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/135910539900400307
[7] Edwards, J. (1746). Religious affections. Grand Rapids, MI: Christian Classics Ethereal Library.
[8] Fenske, M. J., & Eastwood, J. D. (2003). Modulation of focused attention by faces expressing emotion: Evidence from Flanker tasks. Emotion, 3, 327-343.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.3.4.327
[9] Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300-319.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300
[10] Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). Positive emotions. In T. J. Mayne, & G. A. Bonnano (Eds.), Emotion: Current issue and future developments (pp. 123-151). New York: Guilford Press.
[11] Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion, 19, 313-332.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930441000238
[12] Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 845-857.
[13] Hofer, A., Siedentopf, C. M., Ischebeck, A., Rettenbacher, M. A., Verius, M., Felber, S., et al. (2006). Gender differences in regional cerebral activity during the perception of emotion: A functional MRI study. NeuroImage, 32, 854-862.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.053
[14] Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 174-180.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006
[15] Idler, E. L. (1987). Religious involvement and the health of the elderly: Some hypotheses and an initial test. Social Forces, 66, 227-238.
[16] Knudsen, E. I. (2007) Fundamental components of attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 30, 57-78.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094256
[17] Koenig, H. G., Moberg, D. O., & Kvale, J. N. (1988). Religious activities and attitudes of older adults in a geriatric assessment clinic. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 36, 362-374.
[18] Koch, K., Pauly, K., Kellermann, T., Seiferth, N. Y., Reske, M. Backes, V., et al. (2007). Gender differences in the cognitive control of emotion: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2744-2754.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.04.012
[19] McCullough, M. E., & Willoughby, B. L. B. (2009). Religion, Self-Regulation, and Self-Control: Associations, Explanations, and Implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 69-93.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014213
[20] Montagne, B., Kessels, R. P. C., Frigerio, E., Haan, E. H. F. de, & Perrett, D. I. (2005). Sex differences in the perception of affective facial expressions: Do men really lack emotional sensitivity? Cognitive Process, 6, 136-141.
[21] Paloma, M. M., & Pendleton, B. F. (1990). Religious domains and general well-being. Social Indicators Research, 22, 255-276.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00301101
[22] Ren, J. Hu, L. Y., Zhang, H. Y., & Huang, Z. H. (2010). Implicit positive emotion counteracts ego depletion. Social Behavior and Personality, 38, 919-928.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2010.38.7.919
[23] Rounding, K., Lee, A., Jacobson, J. A., & Li, J. (2012). Religion replenishes self-control. Psychological Science, 23, 635-642.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797611431987
[24] Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 383-388.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0605198104
[25] Scholten, M. R. M., Aleman, A., Montagne, B., & Kahn, R. S. (2005). Schizophrenia and processing of facial emotions: Sex matters. Schizophrenia, Research, 78, 61-67.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2005.06.019
[26] Tang, Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., et al. (2007). Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 17152-17156.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0707678104
[27] Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. (2007). Restoring the self: Positive affect helps improve self-regulation following ego depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 379-384.
[28] Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1999). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings, Technical Report A-4. Gainesville, Florida: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology.
[29] Yang, W. (2011). The influence of Tibetan Buddhism on Tibetans’ social mental state and behaviour. Journal of Southwest University for Nationalities (Humanities and Social Science), 33, 17-23.