PSYCH  Vol.3 No.11 , November 2012
Mortality Salience and Metabolism: Glucose Drinks Reduce Worldview Defense Caused by Mortality Salience
ABSTRACT
The current work tested the hypothesis that a glucose drink would reduce worldview defense following mortality salience. Participants consumed either a glucose drink or placebo, wrote about either death or dental pain, and then completed a measure of worldview defense (viewing positively someone with pro-US views and viewing negatively someone with anti-US views). Mortality salience increased world- view defense among participants who consumed a placebo but not among participants who consumed a glucose drink. Glucose might reduce defensiveness after mortality salience by increasing the effectiveness of the self-controlled suppression of death-related thought, by providing resources to cope with mortality salience and reducing its threatening nature, or by distancing the individual from actual physical death.

Cite this paper
Gailliot, M. (2012). Mortality Salience and Metabolism: Glucose Drinks Reduce Worldview Defense Caused by Mortality Salience. Psychology, 3, 991-996. doi: 10.4236/psych.2012.311149.
References
[1]   Anderson, M. C., & Green, C. (2001). Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature, 6826, 366-369. doi:10.1038/35066572

[2]   Aries, P. (1981). The hour of our death. New York: Oxford University Press.

[3]   Arndt, J., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Simon, L. (1997). Suppression, accessibility of death-related thoughts, and cul tural worldview defense: Exploring the psychodynamics of terror management. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 5 18. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.5

[4]   Baumeister, R. F. (2005). The cultural animal: Human nature, meaning, and social life. New York: Oxford.

[5]   Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1994). Losing control: How and why people fail at self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

[6]   Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). Strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 351-355. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x

[7]   Becker, E. (1973). The denial of death. New York: Academic Press.

[8]   Blascovich, J., & Mendes, W. B. (2000). Challenge and threat apprais als: The role of affective cues. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect insocial cognition (pp. 59-82). New York: Cambridge.

[9]   Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1996). The biopsychosocial model of arousal regulation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 1-51. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60235-X

[10]   Cleeland, C. S., Bennett, G. J., Dantzer, R., Dougherty, P. M., Dunn, A. J., et al. (2003). Are the symptoms of cancer and cancer treatment due to a shared biologic mechanism? A cytokine-immunologic mo del of cancer symptoms. Cancer, 97, 2919-2925. doi:10.1002/cncr.11382

[11]   DeWall, C. N., & Baumeister, R. F. (2006). Alone but feeling no pain: Effects of social exclusion on physical pain tolerance and pain threshold, affective forecasting, and interpersonal empathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 1-15. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.1

[12]   DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., Gailliot, M. T., & Maner, J. K. (2008). Depletion makes the heart grow less helpful: Helping as a function of self-regulatory energy and genetic relatedness. Personal ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1653-1662. doi:10.1177/0146167208323981

[13]   DeWall, C. N., Deckman, T., Gailliot, M. T., & Bushman, B. J. (2009). Sweetenedblood cools hot tempers: Physiological self-control and aggression. AggressiveBehavior, 37, 73-80. doi:10.1002/ab.20366

[14]   Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: The neurocognitive overlap between physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 294-300. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.010

[15]   Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290 292. doi:10.1126/science.1089134

[16]   Fairclough, S. H., & Houston, K. (2004). A metabolic measure of men tal effort. Biological Psychology, 66, 177-190. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2003.10.001

[17]   Feifel, H, & Branscomb, A. B. (1973). Who’s afraid of death? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 81, 282-288. doi:10.1037/h0034519

[18]   Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1997). Fear of death and the judgment of social transgressions: A multidimensional test of terror manage ment theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 369 380. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.2.369

[19]   Friese, M., & Hofmann, W. (2008). What would you have as a last supper? Thoughts about death influence evaluation and consumption of food products. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1388-1394. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.06.003

[20]   Gailliot, M. T., Peruche, B. M., Plant., E. A., & Baumeister, R. F. (2009). Stereotypes and prejudice in the blood: Sucrose drinks re duce prejudice and stereotyping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 288-290. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.09.003

[21]   Gailliot, M. T. (2008). Unlocking the energy dynamics of executive functioning: Linking executive functioning to brain glycogen. Per spectives on Psychological Science, 3, 245-263. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00077.x

[22]   Gailliot, M. T. (2009). The effortful and energy-demanding nature of prosocial behavior. In M. Mikulincer, & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Prosocial motives, feelings, and behavior—The better angels of our nature. Washington DC: American Psychological Association.

[23]   Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). The physiology of will power: Linking blood glucose to self-control. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 303-327. doi:10.1177/1088868307303030

[24]   Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M., Brewer, L. E., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Self control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 325-336. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.325

[25]   Gailliot, M. T., Hildebrandt, B., Eckel, L. A., & Baumeister, R. F. (2010). A theory oflimited metabolic energy and premenstrual syndrome (PMS) symptoms—Increased metabolic demands during the luteal phase divert metabolic resourcesfrom and impair self control. Review of General Psychology, 14, 269-282. doi:10.1037/a0018525

[26]   Gailliot, M. T., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2006). Self-regulatory processes defend against the threat of death: Effects of self-control depletion and trait self-control on thoughts and fears of dying. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 49-62. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.49

[27]   Gailliot, M. T., Schmeichel, B. J., & Maner, J. K. (2007). Differentiat ing the effects of self-control and self-esteem on reactions to mortal ity salience. Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 43, 894-901. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.10.011

[28]   Gilliland, M. W. (1978). Energy analysis: A new public policy tool. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

[29]   Goldenberg, J. L., Arndt, J., Hart, J., & Brown, M. (2005). Dying to be thin: The effects of mortality salience and body mass index on restricted eating among women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1400-1412. doi:10.1177/0146167205277207

[30]   Gordijn, E. H., Hindriks, I., Koomen, W., Dijksterhuis, A., & Van Knippenber, A. (2004). Consequences of stereotype suppression and internal suppression motivation: A self-regulation approach. Person ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 212-224. doi:10.1177/0146167203259935

[31]   Greenberg, J., Arndt, J., Schimel, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (2001). Clarifying the function of mortality salience-induced world view defense: Renewed suppression or reduced accessibility of death-related thoughts? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 70-76.

[32]   Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A., Veeder, M., Kirkland, S., & Lyon, D. (1990). Evidence for terror management theory II: The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 308-318. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.308

[33]   Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Simon, L., & Breus, M. (1994). Role of consciousness and accessibility of death-related thoughts in mortality salience effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 627-637. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.627

[34]   Greenberg, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Chatel, D. (1992). Terror management and tolerance: Does mortality salience always intensify negative reactions to others who threaten one’s worldview? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 212 220.

[35]   Harmon-Jones, E., Simon, L., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & McGregor, H. (1997). Terror management theory and self-es teem: Evidence that increased self-esteem reduces mortality salience effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 24-36. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.1.24

[36]   Heine, S. J., Harihara, M., & Niiya, Y. (2002). Terror management in Japan. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 5, 187-196. doi:10.1111/1467-839X.00103

[37]   Hirschberger, G., & Ein-Dor, T. (2005). Does a candy a day keep the death thoughts away? The terror management function of eating. Ba sic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 179-186. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp2702_9

[38]   Kleiber, M. (1961). The fire of life: An introduction to animal energetics. New York: Krieger.

[39]   Landau, M. J., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., Cohen, F., Pyszczynski, T., Arndt, J. et al. (2004). Deliver us from evil: The effects of mortality salience and reminders of 9/11 on support for President George W. Bush. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1136-1150. doi:10.1177/0146167204267988

[40]   Lotka, A. J. (1922). Contribution to the energetics of evolution. Pro ceedings of the National Academy of Science, 8, 147-151. doi:10.1073/pnas.8.6.147

[41]   MacDonald, G., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 202-223. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.202

[42]   MacLeod, C. M. (1989). Directed forgetting affects both direct and indirect tests of memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 13-21. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.15.1.13

[43]   Mandel, N., & Smeesters, D. (2008). The sweet escape: Effects of mortality salience on consumption quantities for high and low-self esteem consumers. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 309-323. doi:10.1086/587626

[44]   Maner, J. K., Kenrick, D. T., Neuberg, S. L., Becker, D. V., Robertson, B., Hofer, B., Delton, A., Butner, J., & Schaller, M. (2005). Functional projection: How fundamental social motives can bias interpersonal perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 63-78. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.63

[45]   Maner, J. K., Gailliot, M. T., & DeWall, C. N. (2007). Adaptive atten tional attunement: Evidence for mating-related perceptual bias. Evo lution and Human Behavior, 28, 28-36. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.05.006

[46]   Masicampo, E. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2008). Toward a physiology of dual-process reasoning and judgment: Lemonade, willpower, and expensive rule-based analysis. Psychological Science, 19, 255-260. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02077.x

[47]   Mayer, J. D., & Gaschke, Y. N. (1988). The experience and meta experience of mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 102-111. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.55.1.102

[48]   McBride, D. M., & Dosher, B. A. (1997). A comparison of forgetting in an implicit and explicit memory task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 371-392. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.126.4.371

[49]   Meyers, C. A., Albitar, M., & Estey, E. (2005). Cognitive impairment, fatigue, and cytokine levels in patients with acute myelogenous leu kemia or myelodysplastic syndrome. Cancer, 104, 788-793. doi:10.1002/cncr.21234

[50]   Meyers, C. A., Byrne, K. S., & Komaski, R. (1995). Cognitive deficits in patients with small cell lung cancer before and after chemotherapy. Lung Cancer, 12, 231-235. doi:10.1016/0169-5002(95)00446-8

[51]   Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and deple tion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psy chological Bulletin, 126, 247-259. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.247

[52]   Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Person ality and Social Psychology, 74, 774-789. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.774

[53]   Ochsmann, R., & Mathey, M. (1994). Depreciating of and distancing from foreigners: Effects of mortality salience. Unpublished manuscript, Mainz: Universitat Mainz.

[54]   Odum, H. T. (1995). Self-organization and maximum empower. In C. A. S. Hall (Ed.), Maximum Power: The Ideas and Applications of H. T. Odum. Colorado: Colorado University Press.

[55]   Pollak, J. M. (1980). Correlates of death anxiety: A review of empirical studies. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying, 10, 97-121. doi:10.2190/4KG5-HBH0-NNME-DM58

[56]   Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (1999). A dual process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: An extension of terror management theory. Psychological Review, 106, 835-845. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.835

[57]   Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Lyon, D. (1989). Evidence for terror management theory I: The effects of mor tality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 681-690. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.681

[58]   Schoen, R. E., Tangen, C. M., Kuller, L. H., Burke, G. L., Cushman, M., Tracy, R. P. et al. (1999). Increased blood glucose and insulin, body size, and incident colorectal cancer. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 91, 1147-1154. doi:10.1093/jnci/91.13.1147

[59]   Smart, L., & Wegner, D. M. (1999). Covering up what can’t be seen: Concealable stigma and mental control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 474-486. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.3.474

[60]   Weber, W. A., Petersen, V., Burkhard, S., Tyndale-Hines, L., Link, T., Peschel, C., & Schwaiger, M. (2003). Positron emission tomography in non-small-cell lung cancer: Prediction of response to chemotherapy by quantitative assessment of glucose use. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 21, 2651-2657. doi:10.1200/JCO.2003.12.004

[61]   Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34-52. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.1.34

[62]   Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression. Journal of Personality, 62, 615-640. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1994.tb00311.x

[63]   Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 59-91. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.59

[64]   Wheatley, T., & Haidt, J. (2005). Hypnotically induced disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychological Science, 16, 780-784. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01614.x

[65]   Younes, M., Lechago, L. V., Somoano, J. R., Mosharaf, M., & Lechago, J. (1996). Wide expression of the human erythrocyte glucose trans porter Glut1 in human cancers. Cancer Research, 56, 1164-1167.

 
 
Top